Tauya (also Inafosa ) is a Rai Coast language spoken in the Ramu River valley, Madang Province , Papua New Guinea by approximately 350 people. The Linguistics Department at the University of Manitoba in Winnipeg , Canada , has Tauya language resources.
29-735: (Redirected from TYA ) Tya or TYA may refer to: Languages [ edit ] Tauya language , a language in Papua New Guinea Places [ edit ] Tin Yat stop , a Light Rail stop in Hong Kong Tya, Sogn og Fjordane , a river in Sogn og Fjordane county, Norway Tya, Trøndelag , a river in Trøndelag county, Norway Other [ edit ] Tya (unit) ,
58-469: A lesser clause to a main one. The vowel may change in sound to a sound that is near by, and rounds the lips when in a word. Tauya allows for the diphthongs ai, au, ae, ao, ou, oi (although oi is rare). Tauya is considered a SOV order language as sentences end strictly with a verb. Some exceptions do exist as some are derivative to the SOV and use the verb to carry the meaning. The Tauya language
87-403: A measure of time meaning "thousands of years ago" Theatre for Young Audiences , a branch of theatre arts attended by or created for younger audiences NordStar , ICAO code Topics referred to by the same term [REDACTED] This disambiguation page lists articles associated with the title Tya . If an internal link led you here, you may wish to change the link to point directly to
116-421: A noun phrase which bears an agent thematic relation. As such, the theta role is called the "agent" theta role. This often leads to confusion between the two notions. The two concepts, however, can be distinguished in a number of ways. One common way of thinking about theta roles is that they are bundles of thematic relations associated with a particular argument position ( Carnie 2006 ). Theta roles are stored in
145-586: A particular verb or predicate. For example, the ARG-STR list of the verb give is <NP, NP, PP>. The semantic part of theta roles (i.e. the thematic relations) are treated in a special set of semantic restriction (RESTR) features. These typically express the semantic properties more directly than thematic relations. For example, the semantic relations associated with the arguments of the verb give are not agent, theme and goal, but giver, given, givee. Many approaches to grammar including construction grammar and
174-438: A sentence must contain values for all the grammatical functions mentioned in a-structure. Head-driven phrase structure grammar (HPSG) (for a textbook introduction, see Sag, Wasow & Bender 2005 ) does not use theta roles per se, but divides their property into two distinct feature structures. The number and category are indicated by a feature called ARG-STR. This feature is an ordered list of categories that must cooccur with
203-414: A unique a-structure role The Subject Condition : Every verb must have a SUBJ F-structures are further constrained by the following two constraints which do much of the same labor as the θ-criterion: Coherence requires that every participant in the f-structure of a sentence must be mentioned in a-structure (or in a constituting equation) of a predicate in its clause. Completeness : An f-structure for
232-405: A verb's theta grid . Grids typically come in two forms. The simplest and easiest to type is written as an ordered list between angle brackets. The argument associated with the external argument position (which typically ends up being the subject in active sentences) is written first and underlined. The theta roles are named by the most prominent thematic relation that they contain. In this notation,
261-460: Is a suffixing one and in arguments it is verb agreement is primarily used and case marking is secondarily used. Verb agreement is used to mark singular or plural in arguments . Medial verbs are used to mark subject of a medial clause that is different or the same from verbs in a clause with a relationship to a following clause. Tauya syllable structure consists of an onset consonant of (C)V(V). In plural forms of personal pronouns consonants are
290-630: Is encoded in the Universal Alignment Hypothesis (or UAH ), where the thematic relations are mapped directly into argument position based on the following hierarchy: Agent < Theme < Experiencer < Others. Mark Baker adopted this idea into GB theory in the form of the Uniformity of Theta Assignment Hypothesis (or UTAH ) ( Baker 1988 ). UTAH explains how identical thematic relationships between items are shown by identical structural relationships. A different approach to
319-488: Is marked explicitly by a preposition or a case marking). Themes map to [-r], second themes map to [+o] and non-themes map to [-o]. These features then determine how the arguments are mapped to specific grammatical functions in the sentence. The first [-o] argument is mapped to the SUBJ (subject) relation. If there is no [-o] argument then the first [-r] argument is mapped to the SUBJ relation. If neither of these apply, then you add
SECTION 10
#1732783008535348-458: Is often used interchangeably with the term thematic relations (particularly in mainstream generative grammar—for an exception see Carnie 2006 ). The reason for this is simple: theta roles typically reference thematic relations. In particular, theta roles are often referred to by the most prominent thematic relation in them. For example, a common theta role is the primary or external argument. Typically, although not always, this theta role maps to
377-633: The Biyom Language and is grouped with Tauya in the Rai Coast subgroup. The two languages relate although Biyom is spoken higher up the mountains as they border close to each other and have been found to have some similarities in diction such as the word kaŋgora meaning initiate which was directly borrowed from the Biyom Language. From this it is known that the two languages have had frequent communication as well as intermarriage between
406-555: The Simpler Syntax model ( Culicover & Jackendoff 2005 ) (see also Jackendoff's earlier work on argument structure and semantics, including Jackendoff 1983 and Jackendoff 1990 ) claim that theta roles (and thematic relations) are neither a good way to represent the syntactic argument structure of predicates nor of the semantic properties that they reveal. They argue for more complex and articulated semantic structures (often called Lexical-conceptual structures ) which map onto
435-476: The category associated with the theta role. This mingles theta-theory with the notion of subcategorization . The bottom row gives a series of indexes which are associated with subscripted markers in the sentence itself which indicate that the NPs they are attached to have been assigned the theta role in question. When applied to the sentence [ S [ NP Susan] i gave [ NP the food] j [ PP to Biff] k ]
464-427: The correct match between the number of arguments (typically NPs, PPs, or embedded clauses) and the number of theta roles, the sentence will be ungrammatical or unparseable. Chomsky's formulation ( Chomsky 1981 , p. 36) is: The theta criterion Each argument bears one and only one θ-role, and each θ-role is assigned to one and only one argument. Although it is often not explicitly stated, adjuncts are excluded from
493-527: The correspondence is given in ( Hale & Keyser 1993 ) and ( Hale & Keyser 2001 ), where there are no such things as underlying theta roles or even thematic relations. Instead, the interpretive component of the grammar identifies the semantic role of an argument based on its position in the tree. Lexical-functional grammar (LFG) ( Falk 2001 ) and ( Bresnan 2001 ) is perhaps the most similar to Chomskyan approaches in implementing theta-roles. However, LFG uses three distinct layers of structure for representing
522-477: The final in morphemes, these ones all have a consonant ending of n as seen in sen . The use of consonant in the ending word is followed by a vowel initial in transitive verbs, impersonal verbs and inalienable nouns. Using them to mark objects, experiencers , and possessors inflecting with ~pi suffix. Consist of personal pronouns, deictic pronouns and full nouns as classes of nouns. Differentiated by person and only two numbers occurring in various cases, such as
551-425: The formal device for representing syntactic argument structure —the number and type of noun phrases—required syntactically by a particular verb. For example, the verb put requires three arguments (i.e., it is trivalent ) . The formal mechanism for implementing a verb's argument structure is codified as theta roles. The verb put is said to "assign" three theta roles. This is coded in a theta grid associated with
580-537: The indices mark that Susan is assigned the external theta role of agent/source, the food is assigned the theme role, and to Biff is assigned the goal role. The theta criterion (or θ-criterion ) is the formal device in Government and Binding Theory for enforcing the one to one match between arguments and theta roles. This acts as a filter on the D-structure of the sentence. If an argument fails to have
609-408: The intended article. Retrieved from " https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Tya&oldid=1257983642 " Categories : Disambiguation pages Place name disambiguation pages Hidden categories: Short description is different from Wikidata All article disambiguation pages All disambiguation pages Tauya language Tauya is closely genetically related to
SECTION 20
#1732783008535638-451: The lexical entry for the verb. The correspondence between the theta grid and the actual sentence is accomplished by means of a bijective filter on the grammar known as the theta criterion . Early conceptions of theta roles include Fillmore (1968) (Fillmore called theta roles "cases") and Gruber (1965) . Theta roles are prominent in government and binding theory and the standard theory of transformational grammar. The term "theta role"
667-399: The names of the participant roles associated with a predicate: the predicate may be a verb, an adjective, a preposition, or a noun. If an object is in motion or in a steady state as the speakers perceives the state, or it is the topic of discussion, it is called a theme. The participant is usually said to be an argument of the predicate. In generative grammar , a theta role or θ-role is
696-447: The plus value ([+r] or [+o]) to the feature structure and apply the following mappings: [-o,-r]: SUBJ, [+o, -r]: Object (OBJ), [-o,+r]: prepositional marked oblique (OBL θ ), [+o, +r]: prepositionally marked object (OBJ θ ). These mappings are further constrained by the following constraints: Function argument biuniqueness : Each a-structure role corresponds to a unique f-structure function, and each f-structure function corresponds to
725-434: The relations or functions of arguments: θ-structure, a-structure (argument structure) and f-structure (functional structure) which expresses grammatical relations. These three layers are linked together using a set of intricate linking principles. Thematic relations in the θ-structure are mapped onto a set of positions in the a-structure which are tied to features [+o] (roughly "object") and [±r] (roughly "restricted" meaning it
754-503: The societies. In Tauya, four of the consonants r, k, kʷ,ʔʷ have restrictions to specific parts of word structure. r occurs in the word-medial position of words in the language and k, kʷ occur mostly in the word-initial position. ʔʷ is restricted to the morpheme- initial position. Tauya has five vowels similar to most languages in Papua New Guinea. e vowel follows many linguistic rules of increasing argument from
783-468: The theta criterion. Drawing on observations based in typological cross-linguistic comparisons of languages ( Fillmore 1968 ), linguists in the relational grammar (RG) tradition (e.g. Perlmutter & Postal 1984 ) observed that particular thematic relations and theta roles map on to particular positions in the sentence. For example, in unmarked situations agents map to subject positions, themes onto object position, and goals onto indirect objects. In RG, this
812-475: The theta grid for a verb such as give is < agent , theme, goal>. The other notation (see for example the textbook examples in Haegeman 1994 and Carnie 2006 ) separates the theta roles into boxes, in which each column represents a theta role. The top row represents the names of the thematic relations contained in the theta role. In some work (e.g., Carnie 2006 ), this box also contains information about
841-411: The unmarked absolutive case. Using third person singular, ne personal pronouns can occur with proclitics on transitive or impersonal verbs for marking objects or experiencers and inalienable nouns for possessors. Third person singular is used independently while in inalienable nouns third person objects, experiencers, and possessors are unmarked. Experiencer (linguistics) Theta roles are
#534465